|
|
ynh I would have preferred some/any follow-up examples supporting Milgram's "experiment." It's an attractive notion, but the fact is only a small number of the original 160 even responded, and apparently they barely rate compared to those crucial 3 folks. The reasons for this were not well explained.
I believe that Milgram's experiment and the so called 'six degrees of separation' is being challenged at the moment. First off, his study used a group of people who were not properly chosen for statistical studies purposes - there were only 160, which can be sufficient to amount to a good 'statistical population' but is borderline, particularly with studies of a very large and potentially diverse population. Secondly, they were a reasonably socially homogenous group - overwhelmingly white (and remember, this was the 1960's) and with an average level of education. I remember reading an article in the Times in the last 6 months which suggested that if you study a group from the inner city, socially deprived populations the number of degrees increases enormously.
There are various studies going on at present which are attempting to replicate/improve the experiment. For example, the enviously well named SmallWorld Project:
http://smallworld.sociology.columbia.edu/
Persephone Also in relation to what Fist wrote, there are plenty of examples of "better" products that failed --betamax vs. VHS, for example. I don't think that it's the case that the product has to be intrinsically good, as long are there are other reasons for people's wanting them. Marketers talk about the four Ps --product, price, place (distribution), and promotion. Any of the other Ps could practically trump product... e.g., I'd think that what makes Microsoft's operating system and software "sticky" is a matter of distribution more than anything else.
I agree (Good Lord, am I allowed to say that on Barbelith?). Nike is constantly selling crap product (along with, undoubtedly, good products albeit with dodgy ethics) to my mind because of massive marketing, enormous existing brand presence in the market and huge distribution etc. VHS beat Beatamax for a variety of reasons, partially to do with VHS realising that rental videos (and, allegedly, porn) would power demand, partially to do with price, partially who knows?
All of this I have no problem with, and nor does the Tipping Point. What the book was about was how small things can result in big changes - the above are merely examples of big things having big results (the Betamax 'x' factor being a possible exception - but, hey, maybe it was a small thing having a big effect!)
One last point which interests me - what I call 'social science's uncertainty principle'. How does the very knowledge or discussion of these structures/systems change them? At the very least, does the attempt to use them change them? (OK, the latter is not quite what Heisenberg would see as an uncertainty principle, but you see what I mean.) Would the cool kids in downtown NY have purchased Hush Puppies if they knew they were deliberately being targeted? Would Paul Revere have had as much impact if people in 1776 had been as propaganda savvy as today's population? If this seems far fetched, just think about how the effectiveness of traditional forms of advertising has gone into free-fall in the past 20 years. Why? Because we are becoming mareketing and advertising cynics - hence advertisers increasingly bizarre attempts to woo us with unusual and unexpected stunts and alternative forms of advertising. Anybody notice Dr. Pepper in Spiderman? In 20 years that sort of image might actually turn you off the product, not on. And if marketers try to use the principles set out in the Tipping Point, could this affect their effectiveness? |
|
|